Convention Coverage

Preparing Teachers for the Unexpected

Daniel T. Willingham

A month into her first job at an elementary school, a newly minted teacher encountered a situation her master’s degree in education had never prepared her for: a 7-year-old girl who, seemingly at random, would get up out of her seat and start spinning in a corner of the classroom.

No one in any of her education classes had ever mentioned how to deal with a “spinner,” she told APS Fellow Daniel T. Willingham, a professor of psychology at the University of Virginia, and yet, when Willingham brought her predicament up with the other elementary school teachers he encountered through his work in educational psychology, they knew exactly the behavior she was talking about.

In general, up-and-coming teachers are receiving training that leaves them poorly prepared for unexpected experiences like this one in real classrooms, Willingham said during his APS-David Myers Distinguished Lecture on the Science and Craft of Teaching Psychological Science at the 2018 APS Annual Convention in San Francisco.

“They are going to encounter problems that no one told them they were going to encounter,” he said, “so what do they do at those moments when they have this novel problem?”

Psychological science has been used to inform education in a number of ways, whether by inspiring classroom practices, or evaluating existing practices, Willingham said.

The problem lies in how psychological science is communicated to teachers, he explained.

“We’re training future practitioners as if they’re future researchers,” Willingham said. “It’s interesting, so to some extent they like it … but it’s not really of very high utility.”

Most graduate programs require education majors to take a course in educational psychology, foundations, or child development. Later on, the Praxis II licensing exam taken by most aspiring teachers in the United States expects test takers to have a basic understanding of how learning occurs, but that doesn’t mean the knowledge sticks with them once they’re out in the world as educators.

In a 2017 study led by Kelly MacDonald (University of Houston), over 17,000 educators and members of the general public were asked to evaluate the accuracy of a series of “neuromyths” and factual information about the brain. When presented with common misconceptions about neuroscience (e.g., “classical music improves reasoning” and “dyslexia is seeing letters backwards”), educators accepted over half of the myths as fact, suggesting that, on average, their grasp of psychological science was only 10% more accurate than that of the general public.

Teachers may not be retaining this information, Willingham said, because they’re not putting it to use, and may even find it irrelevant to their work.

In a 2012 survey, the American Federation of Teachers (AFT) — a labor union that serves over 1 million educators in the United States — members’ chief complaint about their own training was its heavy emphasis on theory over application. Most respondents said that their personal experiences, and the experiences of more senior teachers, in the classroom, were of far more practical use to them than anything they were taught as students themselves.

Basic Science Offers Solutions

Willingham said psychological scientists need to identify what they have to offer teachers rather than what they want teachers to know in classroom settings.

The three kinds of statements scientists make about the world — observations, theories, and epistemic assumptions — can be variably helpful to educators, he explained. While researchers need to be interested in all three, teachers should really only be concerned with observations that have risen to the level of empirical generalizations.

“Empirical generalizations are observations that not just have been replicated but have been shown to be highly regular across different contexts, across ages, across materials and so forth,” Willingham said, “and to me this is giving teachers information on what kids are generally like, what kids think, how kids behave, what their emotional life is like.”

Theories, on the other hand, tend to date back decades and to be abstract.

“If you look at a textbook of educational psychology what you’ll see is a few pages that describe Piaget’s theory and then several pages after that describing all the ways in which we’ve had to modify our understanding,” Willingham explained. “It’s hard for me to see why that would be an especially valuable experience for teachers.”

Epistemic assumptions can also cause confusion, he adds. When psychological scientists say “learning is social,” for example, they mean that social factors should be considered when building a theory about learning. It’s easy for teachers, however, to interpret this kind of statement as meaning that children learn best in social situation, when empirically that’s unsupported.

Theories for Researchers vs. Theories for Practitioners

When it comes to creating a mental model of learners, Willingham said, teachers need one theory based on proven empirical generalizations so that they’re not learning and memorizing something that could be disproven later.

“Theories for educators should be boring to researchers,” he said.

A researcher might explain memory, for example, as a complex multistep process involving concepts such as episodic buffering, visual semantics, and episodic long-term memory, whereas a more teacher-friendly model of memory should simply be comprehensible, he suggested.

Without greater follow through, however, even theories designed specifically for practitioners run the risk of being forgotten, he cautioned. Students pursuing a master’s in education usually take a course on educational psychology during their first semester of graduate school, and have little opportunity to revisit that content until they start studying for the licensing exam, he noted.

Standardizing the curriculum within a program to more consistently cover the psychological science of learning could held address this, but would require the cooperation of professors who are used to having full control over what’s taught in their own classrooms.

“If this is actually going to work, and psychological science is actually going to make a difference, this content needs to be revisited in future courses, and that’s a big ask,” Willingham said. “I don’t know about you all, but I’m pretty used to being the royalty in my classroom.”

Willingham concludes that translating theory into actionable information for teachers will require intermediaries with knowledge of both the classroom and the lab.

“It needs to be people who know the research literature very widely and very deeply and they also need to know classrooms,” he said. “If you’re a pure researcher and you haven’t spent time in classrooms, you don’t know what’s going to be useful to a teacher and you’re not going to know how these principals play out.”

Reference

Macdonald, K., Germine, L., Anderson, A., Christodoulou, J., & Mcgrath, L. M. (2017). Dispelling the Myth: Training in Education or Neuroscience Decreases but Does Not Eliminate Beliefs in Neuromyths. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01314

Comments

Professor Willingham is right about the application of psychology to teaching. I would love to discuss this further.


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.