Teaching Current Directions

Teaching Current Directions in Psychological Science

Aimed at integrating cutting-edge psychological science into the classroom, Teaching Current Directions in Psychological Science offers advice and how-to guidance about teaching a particular area of research or topic in psychological science that has been the focus of an article in the APS journal Current Directions in Psychological Science. Current Directions is a peer-reviewed bimonthly journal featuring reviews by leading experts covering all of scientific psychology and its applications and allowing readers to stay apprised of important developments across subfields beyond their areas of expertise. Its articles are written to be accessible to nonexperts, making them ideally suited for use in the classroom.

Visit David G. Myers and C. Nathan DeWall’s new blog “Talk Psych.” Similar to the APS Observer column, the mission of their blog is to provide weekly updates on psychological science.

How Mindfulness Works

Happy Marriages and Healthy Bodies

How Mindfulness Works

By C. Nathan DeWall

Creswell, J. D., & Lindsay, E. K. (2014). How does mindfulness training affect health? A mindfulness stress buffering account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(6), 401–407, doi: 10.1177/0963721414547415

Rick and Bob are stockbrokers, but their similarity ends there. Rick dreads work each day, chain-smokes, and overreacts to minor market fluctuations. Bob is rarely perturbed, enjoys good health, and has learned to acknowledge and accept life’s inevitable ups and downs. Psychological scientists would describe Bob as mindful. And they would be correct in identifying low and high mindfulness as contributors to why Rick’s health flounders and Bob’s health flourishes.

According to J. David Creswell and Emily Lindsay (2014), an understanding of how mindfulness improves health has been shrouded in mystery. They argue that mindfulness buffers people from stress much in the same way a painkiller dampens unpleasant sensations. But instead of numbing people, mindfulness causes people to take notice of what is happening and accept it — regardless of whether it is a positive, negative, or neutral experience.

Mindful people remain unfazed by stressors for two reasons. First, they swiftly recruit brain regions that aid emotion regulation and show less activity in brain regions such as the amygdala that guide stressful responses (Creswell, Way, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2007; Way, Creswell, Eisenberger, & Lieberman, 2010). Second, their sympathetic-adrenal-medullary (SAM) and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axes do not go into overdrive when stressors arise. Mindful people recognize and accept stress, which buffers them from increased heart rate or excessive secretion of the stress hormone cortisol (Brown, Weinstein, & Creswell, 2012; Nyklíček, Mommersteeg, Van Beugen, Ramakers, & Van Boxtel, 2013). Being shielded from stress can pay tremendous health dividends, such as a lower risk for substance abuse relapse and slowed progression of HIV-related CD4+ T lymphocyte loss (Bowen et al., 2014; Creswell, Myers, Cole, & Irwin, 2009).

To bring this cutting-edge science into the classroom, Creswell uses an activity he calls “Meet Bill.” A series of animated PowerPoint slides illustrate how a little mindfulness produces sweeping changes in how Bill experiences a stressful situation. The first slide, titled “Meet Bill,” provides a caricature of how low levels of mindfulness lead a stressor to disrupt Bill’s general well-being and empathy. The second slide, titled “Meet Mindful Bill,” shows how boosting Bill’s mindfulness creates healthier responses to the same stressor. “By noticing that he’s feeling anger and that his sympathetic nervous system has revved up,” Creswell says, “[Bill] can more effectively engage in regulatory efforts. In this scenario, he takes a couple of deep breaths to bring his heart rate down.”

Instructors can begin by defining mindfulness as “monitoring one’s present-moment experience with acceptance” (Creswell & Lindsay, 2014, p. 402). Remind students that although some people are more mindful than others, mindfulness levels fluctuate from month to month, week to week, and even minute to minute. Next, ask students to form pairs and discuss their responses to the following questions:

  • What was a stressful situation you experienced recently?
  • How do you think that situation affected your physical and mental health?
  • How did it affect your relationships? How would you rate your levels of mindfulness when handling that situation? Based on what you learned from Creswell and Lindsay (2014), how would increasing your mindfulness levels influence how you handled that situation?

The second activity, titled “When Do We Need Mindfulness?,” urges students to think critically about when people will benefit the most from mindfulness training. Yes, mindfulness generally benefits physical and mental health. But Creswell and Lindsay (2014) are careful to point out that “mindfulness training interventions are unlikely to have much impact on health outcomes in low-stress participant groups” (p. 402).

To determine whether a mindfulness-training intervention will buffer people from illness, begin by reviewing the literature that links stressful relationships to reduced immune-system function (Jaremka, Glaser, Malarkey, & Kiecolt-Glaser, 2013). Instructors can then show students the following scenarios on PowerPoint slides:

Slide 1

Jamie and Mark have been married for 2 years. They have similar interests, enjoy spending time together, and resolve conflict healthfully. Unlike many of their friends, they still feel passionate love for each other. They rarely feel sick enough to take medicine or see a doctor.

Slide 2

Rebecca and Pat have dated for the past 5 years. They have known each other since childhood, but they don’t have much in common. Rebecca prefers quiet environments. Pat goes out with friends every weekend to dance clubs. Rebecca and Pat have awful arguments. Rebecca blames Pat for bringing home germs from clubs. She is sure that is why she always has a cold.

Slide 3

Andy and Daniel were recently married. They returned from their Caribbean honeymoon and completed all of the necessary paperwork to adopt their first child. They spend their weekends painting the baby’s room, watching movies, and planning the rest of their lives together. Daniel has always had severe allergies. He goes to the hospital twice a week to get allergy shots.

Ask students to rate how much each couple would benefit from a mindfulness-training program (on a scale from 1, not at all, to 7, extremely). Why did their ratings differ? How would a mindfulness-training program influence each person’s physical and mental health? According to Creswell and Lindsay (2014), Rebecca and Pat are the only couple that would benefit from a mindfulness-training intervention.

Mindfulness would not benefit the other couples because they are not in need of being buffered from stress. Mindfulness provides people with a sort of psychological Kevlar. In times of stress, people feel the urge to fight or flee. Mindfulness offers a healthy third option, in which people do no shy away from conflict or stress. Instead, they embrace it as another part of their daily experience. Protected against the downside of daily drudgery and stress, mindful people are free to flourish.

Happy Marriages and Healthy Bodies

By David G. Myers

Robles, T. F. (2014). Marital quality and health: Implications for marriage in the 21st century. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(6), 427–432, doi: 10.1177/0963721414549043

As social animals, people need people. Like our distant ancestors who lived, hunted, and found protection in groups, we need to belong. We flourish when socially connected in close relationships.

In epidemiological studies tracking thousands of lives over years, people have been at less risk for ill health and premature death when supported by close relationships. When Brigham Young University researchers aggregated data from 148 studies worldwide, they found that those with ample social connections had survival rates (across the average 7.5-year study period) about 50% greater than those with meager connections (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, 2010).

(Smith, Marsden, Hout, & Kim, n.d.; N=52,340)

(Smith, Marsden, Hout, & Kim, n.d.)

Marriage is the prototypical close relationship. Does it predict emotional and physical well-being? In the General Social Survey of Americans, since 1972, 40% of married adults have declared themselves “very happy,” as have 23% of never-married adults (Fig. 1).

Although marital status is a very strong happiness predictor, marital quality is an even better predictor. In the General Social Survey, nearly 6 in 10 people who rated their marriage as “very happy” assessed their lives as a whole as “very happy,” compared with only 12% of those with “pretty happy” marriages and 6% of those with “unhappy” marriages. Happy marriages predict happy lives, and miserable marriages predict miserable lives.

Does marital quality similarly predict physical well-being? Some well-publicized individual studies have hinted “yes.” George Vaillant (2002) found that at age 50, healthy aging was better predicted by a good marriage than by a low cholesterol level. On the flip side, in 32 studies of 6.5 million people, divorce has predicted poor health (Sbarra, Law, & Portley, 2011).

More evidence comes from a massive meta-analysis of all available research on the association between marital quality and physical health (Robles, Slatcher, Trombello, & McGinn, 2014), which Theodore Robles (2014) helpfully summarizes in plain English. The bottom line: The association, though modest, “is similar to that of associations between health behaviors (diet, physical activity) and health outcomes” (p. 427).

The finding holds across a variety of marriage-quality measures (both self-report and behavioral observation) and health measures (including self-report, physician ratings, and biological markers such as blood pressure and cholesterol).

But, instructors may ask, does an association (correlation) between marriage quality and health indicate that the former is causing the latter? Not necessarily. Given the impossibility of experimentally manipulating marriage quality, how would you know? Robles offers a clue: “In longitudinal studies, marital quality generally predicts [health], rather than the reverse” (p. 428).

An obvious follow-up question for class discussion is, “What factors might mediate and explain an effect of marital quality on health?”Students could surely brainstorm some of the possibilities that Robles identifies:

  • Biological mediators: Cardiovascular, endocrine, and immune systems respond to marital strain.
  • Social-cognitive mediators: How you think about your spouse, and how your spouse influences your emotional control, affects your vulnerability to anxiety and sadness.
  • Health behavior mediators: Social support enables healthier eating and lifestyles, including more sleep; marital strain may increase unhealthy eating and likelihood of substance abuse.

The dawn of same-sex marriage in most European countries and US states raises a final question for discussion. The studies cited above were conducted on opposite-sex couples. Will the seeming health benefits of marital quality observed in heterosexual marriages extend to gay marriages? Are there reasons to expect (or not expect) such?

References and Further Reading

Bowen, S., Witkiewitz, K., Clifasefi, S. L., Grow, J., Chawla, N., Hsu, S. H., … Larimer, M. E. (2014). Relative efficacy of mindfulness-based relapse prevention, standard relapse prevention, and treatment as usual for substance use disorders: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 71, 547–556.

Brown, K. W., Weinstein, N., Creswell, J. D. (2012). Trait mindfulness modulates neuroendocrine and affective responses to social evaluative threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 2037–2041.

Creswell, J. D., Myers, H. F., Cole, S. W., & Irwin, M. R. (2009). Mindfulness meditation training effects on CD4+ T lymphocytes in HIV-1 infected adults: A small randomized controlled trial. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23, 184–188.

Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 560–565.

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7, e1000316.

Jaremka, L. M., Glaser, R., Malarkey, W. B., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2013). Marital distress prospectively predicts poorer cellular immune function. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 2713–2719.

Nyklíček, I., Mommersteeg, P. M. C., Van Beugen, S., Ramakers, C., Van Boxtel, G. J. (2013). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and physiological activity during acute stress: A randomized controlled trial. Health Psychology, 32, 1110–1113.

Robles, T. F., Slatcher, R. B., Trombello, J. M., & McGinn, M. M. (2014). Marital quality and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 140–187.

Sbarra, D. A., Law, R. W., & Portley, R. M. (2011). Divorce and death: A meta-analysis and research agenda for clinical, social, and health psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(5), 454–474.

Smith, T. W., Marsden, P., Hout, M., & Kim, J. (n.d.) General Social Surveys, 1972–2012. [Data file]. Retrieved from http://www3.norc.org/GSS+Website/

Vaillant, G. E. (2002). Aging well: Surprising guideposts to a happier life from the landmark Harvard study of adult development. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.

Way, B. M., Creswell, J. D., Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dispositional mindfulness and depressive symptomatology: Correlations with limbic and self-referential neural activity during rest. Emotion, 10, 12–24.


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.