Harnessing the Power of the Crowd Could Improve Screening Accuracy

This is a photo of a person screening baggage scans

Averaging the results from two independent participants improved screening accuracy, whether participants were looking at baggage scans or mammograms, according to findings published in Psychological Science, a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

The research findings, reported by researchers at Brunel University, suggest that having multiple screeners could improve the detection of rare items in real-world contexts, such as airport security, radiology and military reconnaissance.

“There is a known problem with detecting rare targets,” said study author Jennifer E Corbett, an honorary lecturer for Brunel’s College of Health and Life Sciences. “When you go to the airport, they always seem to find the bottle of water in your bag – it’s a very common item, so people have a mental template. They’ll just find it. But with rare targets like weapons and guns, people see these far less frequently, so are more likely to miss them.”

This is an animation showing an example of the mammogram screening task

Can you tell which of these mammograms shows a tumor? Check your answers below. Source: Brunel University

Corbett, who coauthored the paper with Brunel researcher Jaap Munneke, says the problem lies with the human visual system, which is only capable of processing a few objects in detail at any given moment. The brain averages out redundant and specific information, filling in the spaces based on prior knowledge. As a result, infrequent objects – those that the observers aren’t expecting to see – are often missed.

However, two people independently looking at the same scan perceive it differently, significantly increasing the possibility of infrequent items being spotted.

“We found that when we pair the estimates of two people who don’t know they’re working together – they have no interaction whatsoever – there is a huge improvement in detection, just by capitalizing on the diversity of people’s judgments,” said Corbett.

To test their ideas, Corbett and Munneke conducted two experiments – one which challenged participants to undertake airport screening and the other mammogram screening.

In the airport screening experiment, 16 participants, who had no experience with security screening, saw an image containing nine objects for half a second. They then indicated whether they’d like to call the image back, based on whether they detected a target object.

“The experiment tested weapons detection as well as simple detection tasks,” said Corbett. “We found that not only did pairing observers estimates improve detection in both types of tasks, but that pairing individuals’ estimates from the simple task in a way that maximized the decorrelated patterns actually improved the performance in the separate weapons task.”

The researchers discovered that when they paired the detections of two people who worked individually and independently, they not only saw an increase in the detection of rare objects, but also a reduction in the likelihood of harmless items being wrongly flagged as suspicious.

For the second experiment, 18 participants learned how to identify a tumor on a mammogram. They then saw 400 unique scans, 5% of which had a tumor present, and then another 400, of which 50% had a tumor present.

In both cases, a significant increase in detection rate was observed when two individuals’ results were averaged.

“The task is not so different between airport scanner and a radiologist – the idea is you’re looking for something you have knowledge of but see infrequently,” said Corbett. “It doesn’t matter though whether it’s a tumor or a weapon or something else, averaging two different perceptions of the same scene increases detection.”

The researchers say their detection method is a marked improvement over those currently used in airport and radiological screening, as it significantly reduces the time someone needs to look at a scan.

“The method we propose is probably the best candidate for maximizing the resources of a limited pool of highly trained experts needing to detect rare targets in a lot of images,” said Corbett. “Obviously the limit here is that it requires a second set of eyes, but we’re now looking for ways to use a deep-learning algorithm to cover the aspects of the images which are causing these decorrelations. We can then pair a single person with the algorithm.”

All data have been made publicly available via the Open Science Framework. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this article is available online. This article has received the badge for Open Data.

This image shows the answers to the sample mammogram screening task

Correct answers. Source: Brunel University


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.