Hitting the Streets for Science: What Motivates Protest Behavior?

This past weekend tens of thousands of people participated in science marches in more than 600 cities around the world. Scientists on all seven continents — even Antarctica – participated in the protest.

At a March for Science press conference, Rush Holt, CEO of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, noted that “Scientists are often reticent to go public.”

And, as it turns out, scientists aren’t unusual in this regard. According to the World Values Survey, fewer than one in five citizens of North America, Western Europe, Australia, and New Zealand report having participated in a political demonstration; more than a third say they would “never” do so.

So what are the circumstances that get people engaged in protests like the March for Science? In a recent article in Current Directions in Psychological Science, an international team of researchers from the United States, Germany, and New Zealand analyzed the key factors that spark the motivation for political protest.

“Given the extent of inequality and unfairness in the world, it is surprising how few people actually protest,” authors John T. Jost, Julia Becker, Danny Osborne, and Vivienne Badaan write.

Social-psychological models of collective action have identified three key antecedents of protest: anger at perceived injustice, social identification, and beliefs about group efficacy. That is, “collective action is more likely when people have shared interests, feel relatively deprived, are angry, believe they can make a difference, and strongly identify with relevant social groups,” Jost and co-authors explain.

However, these models rarely account for ideological factors that might motivate – or prevent – people from engaging in protests. People’s emotions are directed not only at individuals and groups but also at larger social systems. According to system-justification theory, most people are motivated to justify and defend the social, economic, and political systems on which they depend – even when these systems may be unfair or unjust. Protests occur in contexts where people are motivated to defend this structural status quo, or when they’re motivated to challenge and oppose it.

In the case of the March for Science, recent threats to science have generated enough moral outrage amongst scientists to elicit a forceful backlash against the status quo — as some protest signs put it, “I’m not a mad scientist, I’m absolutely furious.”

“Whether one likes it or not, there is, in fact, an ideological element to the protest that one appreciates only by taking into account the broader sociopolitical context (or system): I am talking here about a political context in which conservative opposition to scientific methods and findings sets the stage, along with near-constant threats to further reduce or eliminate federal funding for scientific research,” Jost explained. “Thus, those who are participating in the [March for Science] are questioning or challenging a status quo in which science is deprioritized in our society, relative to many other things. People who are willing to march in order to change governmental priorities are therefore likely to be more tolerant of uncertainty, insecurity, and social discord, in comparison with the average person. They are, in other words, low system-justifiers, at least with respect to the government.”

Jost and colleagues note that system justification can also undermine willingness to protest, even among political activists.

For instance, May Day protestors in Greece primed with a system-justifying stereotype exhibited less anger and less willingness to protest compared to a control group of protestors. In another study, members of a British teachers’ union primed with a system-rejecting mind-set scored lower on system justification and exhibited more anger at the government and more willingness to protest than those assigned to a control condition. The effect of system justification on willingness to engage in non-disruptive protest — writing letters, signing petitions, attending meetings, and distributing leaflets — was also mediated by a reduction in anger toward the government.

“In this case, those who are protesting the national government’s deprioritization of science would be considered low system-justifiers (and are relatively tolerant of uncertainty, insecurity, and social discord),” Jost said. “No doubt there will also be an angry backlash of some kind by high system-justifiers (who are relatively intolerant of uncertainty, insecurity, and social discord) — people who are offended by the actions of protesters.”

It’s impossible to know whether governments will change their tune on the importance of science, but one result of the marches may be an increased enthusiasm for protest among March for Science participants.

“[T]here is evidence that after participating in a single protest event, people come to think of themselves as the kind of person who protests against certain kinds of injustices, and this makes them more likely to participate in protest later,” Jost said.

John T. Jost will be part of a symposium honoring 2016 William James Fellow Award recipient Mahzarin Banaji at the 29th APS Annual Convention being held May 25-28 in Boston, USA.

 

Reference

Jost, J. T., Becker, J., Osborne, D., & Badaan, V. (2017). Missing in (Collective) Action: Ideology, System Justification, and the Motivational Antecedents of Two Types of Protest Behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(2), 99-108. doi: 10.1177/0963721417690633


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.