Studying People in Their Local Environments

Psychological science is ostensibly interested in the behavior and mental processes of a variety of people, not just processes related to college students and Mechanical Turk workers responding to surveys over the internet. Gaining the generalizable knowledge we seek requires studying a variety of people in a variety of contexts; it is the only way to know whether and how generalizable our findings really are.

And if we want our research to inform social policies, then it should include a broader composition of people and situations. Only then can we understand how policies that are generated from our findings might differentially affect individuals and subgroups.

Vehicles to Move Us Forward

The implications I’ve just outlined are the main reasons my colleagues and I have been shifting our scientific work to include more mobile research methods. In the Department of Communication at Cornell University, we have a mobile research lab that allows us to diversify both the samples and settings in which we conduct social scientific research. The lab was originally funded by the National Institutes of Health, specifically for the purposes of including diverse and hard-to-reach populations in research on health messaging.

The lab is the size of a small RV and is fully equipped with five private data collection stations. It enables us to recruit and study people in their own environments. To date, my colleagues and I have used it to study youth and adults from rural and urban settings throughout the Northeast region of the United States. For one set of studies, my colleagues wanted to examine how socioeconomically disadvantaged youth and adult smokers respond to different kinds of warning labels on cigarette packages. So they took the lab to a variety of urban and rural communities to recruit participants. They not only learned about how individuals attended to and processed the labels (via eye-tracking and surveys), but also how living in those different environments affects people’s smoking decisions. These lessons are important for both the science and any policies that result from it.

My own ongoing research with the mobile lab is combining a variety of methodological techniques including geographic air-quality mapping, eye-tracking, and surveys to examine how people make sense of, and are motivated to respond to, information about the environmental health hazards in their surroundings. My collaborator in systems engineering used transportation data to develop fine-grained maps depicting levels of air pollution in different neighborhoods of the greater New York City area. Since we have rich information about people’s differential exposure to pollutants, as well as data about the demographic composition of neighborhoods, we have been taking the lab to neighborhoods of differential exposure and composition to examine how residents of those neighborhoods respond to the information as functions of both their individual characteristics and features of their local environments. Conducting the study in this way allows us to learn about relevant psychological processes in an ecologically valid way.

As researchers, the mobile lab has had several benefits. First, it has forced us to think more critically about factors that influence participants, and their implications for both the theories we can advance and any practical knowledge generated from our work. It provides vivid reminders that people are embedded in broader ecological systems and that we must think carefully about how multiple dimensions of those systems interact with individuals when developing our models. That modeling and theorizing inevitably leads to important discussions about measurement, construct validity, and generalizability across samples and settings.

Human Rights Considerations

Using the mobile lab has also reminded us of another important lesson. In the 1948 Universal Declaration of Human Rights, the United Nations General Assembly declared that “everyone has the right freely to…share in scientific advancement and its benefits” (UNESCO, 2005). When we take the mobile lab into (particularly underserved) communities, it provides opportunities for people to exercise that human right — to have their perspectives reflected in the scientific record … a record that often influences the policies and practices that govern their society. That reality is not lost on our participants. I have been deeply moved by people’s gratitude for being allowed to participate in our study because, to use their words, “no one usually cares what [they] think.” It is as if they have learned from scientists that, to borrow from The Op-Ed Project (2017), “some people narrate the world; other people have their world narrated for them;” that there are some people who get to be part of the scientific record and others who do not.

To quote Audrey Squire (2015), “historical exclusivity often has a way of turning into present and institutionalized tragedy. Whose story gets told matters.” What I’ve learned from using the mobile lab is that when we make decisions about who to include in our studies, and which environments to study, we are (implicitly) making decisions about whose psychological processes matter to us. And those decisions have important implications for the knowledge we create, and the policies and practices that are developed as a result of that knowledge.

The mobile lab was co-funded by the National Institutes of Health, Cornell College of Agriculture and Life Sciences, and College of Human Ecology; some of the ongoing research described in this article is funded by the David R. Atkinson Center for a Sustainable Future.

References

Brick, C., Freeman, A. L. J., Wooding, S., Skylark, W. J., Marteau, T. M., & Spiegelhalter, D. J. (2018). Winners and losers: communicating the potential impact of policies. Palgrave Communications, 4(69). doi:10.1057/s41599-018-0121-9.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.

Byrne, S., Greiner Safi, A., Kemp, D., Skurkca, C., Davydova, J., Scolere, L., …, & Niederdeppe, J. (2017). Effects of varying color, imagery, and text of cigarette package warning labels among socioeconomically disadvantaged middle school youth and adult smokers. Health Communication. doi:10.1080/10410236.2017.1407228.

Flake, J. K., Pek, J., & Hehman, E. (2017). Construct validation in social and personality research: Current practice and recommendations. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(4), 370-378.

Fried, E. I., & Flake, J. K. (2018). Measurement matters. Observer, 31(3).

Giner-Sorolla, R. (2019). From crisis of evidence to a “crisis” of relevance? Incentive-based answers for social psychology’s perennial relevance worries. European Review of Social Psychology, 30, 1-38.

Goroff, D. L., Lewis, N. A., Jr., Scheel, A. M., Scherer, L. D., & Tucker, J. A. (2018, November 1). The Inference Engine: A grand challenge to address the context sensitivity problem in social science research. doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/j8b9a

Rohrer, J. M. (2018). Thinking clearly about correlations and causation: Graphical causal models for observational data. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(1), 27-42.

Simons, D. J., Shoda, Y., & Lindsay, D. S. (2017). Constraints on generality (COG): A proposed addition to all empirical papers. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6), 1123-1128.

Skitka, L. J., & Sargis, E. G. (2006). The internet as psychological laboratory. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 529-555.

Squire, A. (2015, February 4). MLK’s mother was assassinated, too: The forgotten women of Black History Month. Talking Points Memo. Retrieved from: https://talkingpointsmemo.com/cafe/mlk-mother-was-assassinated-forgotten-women-black-history-month

Teachman, B. A., Norton, M. I., & Spellman, B. A. (2015). Memos to the President from a “Council of Psychological Science Advisers.” Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(6), 697-700.

The Op-Ed Project (2017, December 2). Write to change the world. New York, NY. UNESCO.

UNESCO (2005). Universal declaration on bioethics and human rights. Retrieved from: http://www.unesco.org/new/en/social-and-human-sciences/themes/bioethics/bioethics-and-human-rights/

Whipple, S. S., Evans, G. W., Barry, R. L., & Maxwell, L. E. (2010). An ecological perspective on cumulative school and neighborhood risk factors related to achievement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 422-427.

Whitsett, D. D., & Shoda, Y. (2014). An approach to test for individual differences in the effects of situations without using moderator variables. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 94-104.

Comments

I can see how realistic the findings of researches from this kind of methods.Actually, the univesrity where I work or in any other institutions in the Philippines are still stock on the idea of using quanti or quali or a combination of the two designs as far as Psychology is concern. I learned from you that we should put emphasis as well on the ecological system and practical ways of collecting data that uniquely represents the beliefs of hard-to-reach populations.Thanks for this innovation.

” .. the United Nations General Assembly declared that “everyone has the right freely to…share in scientific advancement and its benefits” (UNESCO, 2005). When we take the mobile lab into (particularly underserved) communities, it provides opportunities for people to exercise that human right — to have their perspectives reflected in the scientific record.”
-In order to exercise that right fully, you should at least provide your participants a link to the final rapport of your study.

I find that the method of application of a mobile research laboratory is useful to study people in their own environment, always adapting to the needs of each population at a given time.


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.