Up Front

Mind, Body, Illness: Amidst Pandemic, Opportunities for Discovery

This is my final column to you as APS President. In the midst of this extraordinarily difficult and trying time, I hope that you and your loved ones remain healthy and safe.

In early March, as I raced home from abroad to shelter in place with my family, I asked myself a question: Are respiratory diseases, such as the common cold, physical illnesses or psychological illnesses? If you think this is a silly question, think again. When scientists place a cold virus directly into the noses of healthy adults, only about one-third develop respiratory infections. So a cold must have other causal factors. And some of them may be psychological.

The Common Cold Project is a series of prospective viral-challenge studies that were conducted from 1986 to 2011 (e.g., Cohen, 2005). Psychologist Sheldon Cohen and colleagues (Cohen, Miller, & Rabin, 2001; Cohen et al., 2013) placed viruses into the noses of healthy adults, including a coronavirus linked to the common cold, and observed who developed respiratory infections. Five studies revealed that a virus is not a sufficient cause. Chronic stress, affect and emotion, social isolation, social and economic disadvantage, childhood adversity, or other psychosocial factors may also confer risk. Social integration, social support, positive affect, and a high-quality relationship with parents in childhood may confer resilience.

It’s tempting to assume that a virus is the major cause of a cold, and any psychosocial factors are subsidiary and merely moderate the impact. After all, you cannot develop a cold without a virus. But a virus is not the cause. Respiratory infections, like most biological phenomena, more likely emerge from a complex web of weak, interdependent causal factors—the kind I described in my March 2020 Observer column (see also Lewontin, 2000)—and a virus is one of those causal factors.

See all columns by Lisa Feldman Barrett

Here’s what I mean. A virus does not “replicate itself” from its genes within a passive human body. Its capacity to cause infection requires certain necessary conditions in that body’s cells, immune system, and brain. The ensemble of “brain+body” provides a necessary environment for a virus. It contributes to the likelihood of its own infection, not only by the state of its immune and endocrine systems, but also by the mental phenomena it creates, which in turn are linked to immune and metabolic function. Therefore, the probability and severity of infection at any moment, as well as resilience (exposure to a virus with no symptoms), is a unique confluence of a virus in a body with a thinking, feeling, and perceiving brain. Both virus and “brain+body” are mutually dependent causes; therefore, neither should be ignored in science or in practice.

In this view, psychological factors may be necessary causes of respiratory illness. They are not sufficient to develop a common cold, but then, neither is a virus. Cohen et al.’s studies are consistent with a larger body of important research by health psychologists which suggests again and again that mind and body are linked, not in a vague metaphorical way, but in a real, probabilistic, biological way. What we think and feel, how we experience the world, and who we experience it with may translate into vulnerabilities or resilience to illness (e.g., see Slavich, 2020).

If psychological factors causally contribute to respiratory illness, then scientific research that directly studies these factors could prove vital to physicians, epidemiologists, and virologists on the front lines of the COVID-19 pandemic. This research may, in fact, be as important as the more biologically focused science that fills the front section of major newspapers these days. Reifying an illusory boundary between mind and body, mental and physical, weakens our understanding of disease, and so it’s tragic (and frankly, infuriating) that decisionmakers in this unfolding drama, and the media who report on it, have a blind spot for research that dissolves this boundary, at least as of mid-April when this column went to print.

Historians have shown time and again that improving biomedicine alone, without addressing the psychosocial conditions that foster the spread of respiratory illness, threatens the most vulnerable among us and allows viruses to persist.

This blind spot could arise for various reasons. We don’t know, for example, whether findings from the Common Cold Project generalize to respiratory infection from the COVID-19 virus. Nor do we know whether the findings generalize to a pandemic unfolding in naturalistic settings around the globe. But the Cohen et al. findings provide a solid foundation for hypothesis-testing these and other empirical questions. Similarly, there is research suggesting that chronic stress may impact the effectiveness of vaccines to battle infection, particularly when the body’s immune system is producing antibodies to a novel strain of a virus (i.e., where a person does not have much in the way of preexisting antibodies when exposed; Quigley et al., 2007; Vedhara et al., 1999). And, indeed, psychological stress may blunt the antibody response to vaccines that are developed for newer influenza strains (also a respiratory illness; e.g., Cohen et al., 2001; Pedersen et al., 2009). If these hypotheses are ignored, particularly as the pandemic evolves, we may miss opportunities to make potentially life-saving discoveries.

Another reason for this blind spot may be that, frankly, we don’t know exactly how mental events, like the experience of stress, are transformed into physical conditions, like immune function. But an absence of knowing everything is not the same as knowing nothing. The Common Cold Project suggests that, at least for cold viruses, psychosocial factors leave a brain less able to regulate the proinflammatory cytokine response upon exposure to a virus. Other research has established a relationship between shorter telomere length and chronic stress (e.g., Epel et al., 2004), increases in proinflammatory cytokines, and poorer antibody response to vaccines (e.g., Goronzy et al., 2001; Saurwein-Teissl et al., 2002), and people with shorter telomeres (in white blood cells that help mount an immune response to pathogens) had an increased risk of respiratory infection (Cohen et al., 2013).

Yet another reason: The scientific findings might confuse people and cause inadvertent harm. Governments, medical providers, and scientists in many countries have been largely concerned with flattening the curve (i.e., managing the number of people with infection) so as not to overwhelm medical providers and increase the death rate. A public message that social support offers some protection from illness, for example, might lead people to ignore pleas to socially distance, etc. So alongside the science, we’d also need clear, concrete messaging: Maintain social connections while remaining physically distanced from others. Try to manage your stress and your emotions as you continue to wash your hands, disinfect surfaces, and perform all the other behaviors that reduce your virus exposure.

See all APS Presidential Columns since 1999

Regardless of these concerns, it is very possible that psychological caretaking—reducing stress, decreasing loneliness, and generally managing one’s emotional experiences—could help to flatten the curve after exposure to the virus. Many people, including many scientists, however, find it challenging to grapple with the idea that illness and disease might be caused by a virus plus the psychological conditions of its host, rather than by a virus alone. This brings me to perhaps the most important reason for the blind spot: Many people cling to a belief in biological determinism and simple unitary causes. This pair of beliefs—this ideology of illness (Lewontin, 1991)—lurks in large swaths of medical and scientific practice. In the present pandemic, biological determinism is to assume that COVID-19 has a fixed, deterministic capacity to cause illness, independent of the conditions of its host. A belief in simple, unitary causes is to assume that COVID-19 is responsible for respiratory infection in the normal medical sense and therefore is a sufficient cause, and all other factors (psychological, social, economic) must be modulatory.

This blind spot has serious consequences for the health and welfare of people who are struggling during this pandemic.

One immediate consequence is that people die from infection at a faster rate when they live in conditions that foster chronic stress and other psychological causes that make infection more likely. Other factors play a role here too, such as less access to health care, discriminatory treatment practices, and living conditions that make social distancing a luxury. But historians have shown time and again that improving biomedicine alone, without addressing the psychosocial conditions that foster the spread of respiratory illness, threatens the most vulnerable among us and allows viruses to persist (see McMillen 2016). From the mid-18th century to the beginning of the 19th century, for example, most people in Europe and North America died of infectious respiratory diseases such as tuberculosis, influenza, and pneumonia. Death rates from these illnesses slowly declined as nutrition, housing, and wages improved. The discovery of vaccines and other medicines had an effect, of course, but conditions that reduced chronic stress, allowed parents more quality time to spend with their children, and increased well-being were the major causal factors (Burney, 2000; Lewontin, 1991). Even in the United States today, infant mortality falls with every dollar increase of the minimum wage (Komro et al., 2016).

Our current societal system of social stratification and economic disparity, and the psychological sequelae that it cultivates, belong to the web of factors that caused this pandemic. These consequences are now coming for the rest of us as people lose their jobs in droves. And severe, downstream effects may already be in motion; for example, children of stressed or depressed parents may be more likely to suffer health problems decades down the road (e.g., Hullam et al., 2019; Raposa et al., 2014), perhaps via alterations in immune function that begin in childhood (e.g., Ulmer-Yaniv et al., 2018).

Right now, our society spends substantially more time, creativity, and money on isolated biological or chemical approaches to health and illness than it does to understand how thoughts, feelings, and behaviors create conditions for health or illness to flourish. What would life be like, right now, if people understood that there may be sound biological reasons for reducing stress and negative affect, investing time and energy in social connectedness (particularly for children, the elderly, and people who are most at risk for illness), and generally being more mindful of how they can curate their day-to-day experiences?

We psychological scientists are uniquely positioned to answer these questions through our research and by educating the public. If we follow the lead of Cohen and other health psychologists, and continue to dissolve the porous boundary between the mental and the physical, we can remedy the blind spot and create opportunities for discovery that could ultimately save lives.

References

Burney, P. (2000). Respiratory disease. Epidemiological Reviews, 22, 107–111.

Cohen, S. (2005). The Pittsburg common cold studies: Psychosocial predictors of susceptibility to respiratory infectious illness. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 123–131.

Cohen, S., Miller, G. E., & Rabin, B. S. (2001). Psychological stress and antibody response to immunization: A critical review of the human literature. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 7–18.

Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., Turner, R.B., Casselbrant, M .L., Li-Korotky, H-S., et al. (2013). Association between telomere length and experimentally induced upper respiratory viral infection in healthy adults. Journal of the American Medical Asssociation, 309, 699–705.

Epel, E.S., Blackburn, E.H., Lin, J., Dhabhar, F. S., Adler, N.E., et al. (2004). Accelerated telomere shortening in response to life stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 17312–17315.

Goronzy, J.J., Fulbright, J.W., Crowson, C.S., Poland, G.A., O’Fallon, W.M., & Weyand, C.M. (2001). Value of immunological markers in predicting responsiveness to influenza vaccination in elderly individuals. Journal of Virology, 75, 12182–12187.

Hullam, G., Antal, P., Petschner, P., Gonda, X., Bagdy, G., et al. (2019). The UKB envirome of depression: From interactions to synergistic effects. Scientific Reports, 9, 9723.

Komro, K. A., Livingston, M. D., Markowitz, S., & Wagenaar, A. C. (2016). The effect of an increased minimum wage on infant mortality and birth weight. American Journal of Public Health, 106, 1514–1516.

Lewontin, R. C. (1991). Biology of ideology: The doctrine of DNA. New York: Harper Collins.

Lewontin, R. (2000). The triple helix: Gene, organism and environment. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

McMillen, C. W. (2016). Pandemics: A very short introduction. Oxford University Press.

Pedersen, A. F., Zachariae, R., & Bovbjerg, D. H. (2009). Psychological stress and antibody response to influenza vaccination: A meta-analysis. Brain, Behavior and Immunity, 23, 427–433.

Quigley, K.S., Rodrigues, I.M., Santos, S.L., and Helmer, D.A. (2007, May 25). Negative affect heuristic influences risk and benefit ratings in a vaccination context. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Psychological Science.

Raposa, E., Hammen, C., Brennan, P., & Najman, J. (2014). The long-term effects of maternal depression: Early childhood physical health as a pathway to offspring depression. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54, 88–93.

Saurwein-Teissl, M., Lung, T.L., Marx, F., Gschösser, C., Asch, E., et al., (2002). Lack of antibody production following immunization in old age: Association with CD8(+)CD28(-) T cell clonal expansions and an imbalance in the production of Th1 and Th2 cytokines. Journal of Immunolology, 168, 5893–5899.

Slavich, G. M. (2020). Social safety theory: A biologically based evolutionary perspective on life stress, health and behavior. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045159.

Ulmer-Yaniv, A., Djalovski, A., Priel, A., Zagoory-Sharon, O., & Feldman, R. (2018). Maternal depression alters stress and immune biomarkers in mother and child. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 1145–1157.

Vedhara, K., Cox, N.K.M., Wilcock, G. K., Perks, P., Hunt, M., et al. (1999). Chronic stress in elderly carers of dementia patients and antibody response to influenza vaccine. The Lancet, 353, 627–631.

Comments

Thank you for an interesting piece connecting mind-body. I would encourage us all to be very modest in our claims and very cautious in promoting the role of emotions and stress in this disease and others. We have far too much to learn to propose the emotions play a major direct (vs. emotions leading to poor self care, nutrition, less exercise, etc) role. In my lifetime there were those attempting to make a direct link between stress and cancer and more recently, between stress and ulcers. There are clearly social determinants (poverty and income linked with health care access; nutrition) and beliefs (invulnerability; distrust of government) that play a role that can indeed be impacted by social and psychological interventions. There are too many “pseudoscientists” out there who will take this idea of mind-body are overly promote the connection, not helping those who need help and ultimately damaging the science of psychology.

This is one of the most insanely delusional things I have ever read. No offense. Weaponized advanced pseudoscience gobbledygook.

You really should remove the word science from this, it sullies the very idea and cheapens centuries of painful lessons that created the scientific method.

It’s quite the leap of logic to go from Cohen saying that stress and other psychosocial factors can influence the immune response to saying “psychological factors may be necessary causes of respiratory illness”. That’s akin to saying that air currents are necessary to flip a coin. I assure you, they’re not, though they can certainly influence the outcome.

Similarly, the quote about psychosocial conditions being a factor in spreading respiratory illness says nothing about them being a requisite part of the spread, though this article tries to imply that they are. I suppose, in the extreme case, there would be no disease spread if we had 100% isolation, but that’s simply not going to happen. Psychosocial considerations come into play when we consider how society and individuals will react to different messaging about isolation, where to draw the line, and what laws should be enacted. Different people react very differently to government regulations surrounding that topic.

Finally, I have immense difficulty taking an article seriously that claims to find it infuriating when biological science focuses almost entirely on biology, but then itself focuses almost entirely on psychology, as if that were just as causative in physical disease as the physical aspects. Psychosociology and mental health are very much part of managing both individuals and society during a pandemic, to be certain, but I don’t see any of the quoted studies suggesting that psychology is on the same level as biology in the spread of respiratory illness.

This article has distinct undertones of blaming the victim for not being positive enough, or not doing enough, to fend off their illness. Sadly, that’s not the way viruses work (or chronic illnesses or any of the other cases that this “logic” is often applied to).

Infection is a complex phenomenon and does not arise from simple single causes, like exposure to a virus. So to understand how infection arises, it is necessary to understand the biology of both the virus and the host, and if the host is human, then the biology is linked to that person’s mental life and social experiences. A virus does not “replicate itself” from its genes within a passive human body. Its capacity to cause infection requires certain necessary conditions in that body’s cells, immune system, and brain. This is not “blaming the victim.” The column points to many social factors that impact the psychological well-being of people, and which, therefore, are relevant to the biology of infection.


APS regularly opens certain online articles for discussion on our website. Effective February 2021, you must be a logged-in APS member to post comments. By posting a comment, you agree to our Community Guidelines and the display of your profile information, including your name and affiliation. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations present in article comments are those of the writers and do not necessarily reflect the views of APS or the article’s author. For more information, please see our Community Guidelines.

Please login with your APS account to comment.